Objectivity and Ideology
Criticism of Theo Sandfort’s Research on Man-Boy
Sexual Relations
Robert Bauserman
Journal of Homosexuality, vol. 20, nr. ½, 1990
Summary
Three critiques of Theo Sandfort’s research on man-boy sexual
relationships in the Netherlands are examined and evaluated. Three types
of criticism - methodological, speculative and moral - are identified.
Specific criticisms of the study are evaluated on the basis of their
validity and, where appropriate, their underlying assumptions. It is
argued that moral condemnation of such relationships, combined with a
prevailing ideology of boy "victims" and adult
"perpetrators," results in efforts by Sandfort’s critics to
attack and discredit his research rather than evaluate it objectively.
Article
In 1981 Theo Sandfort, lecturer in psychology at the State University
of Utrecht in the Netherlands, published the results of his study of 25
boys aged 10 to 16 who were involved in ongoing pederastic relationships
with adult men at the time of the study. He concluded that "For
virtually all the boys ... the sexual contact itself was experienced
positively and had no negative effect on how the youngster felt in
general" (Sandfort, 1982, p. 85). Sandfort also determined that the
boys did not perceive a misuse of power by the men. An English translation
of his book appeared in 1982. In 1983, an article by Sandfort entitled
"Pedophile relationships in the Netherlands: Alternative Lifestyles
for Children?" appeared in the Spring 1983 issue of Alternative
Lifestyles. This article was not intended as a full summary of Sandfort’s
study but instead, in the author’s own words, concentrated on "the
meaning of the older partner and the pedophile relationship itself for the
younger partner" (Sandfort, 1983, p. 165). In 1984, an article
providing a fuller summary of Sandfort’s research appeared in the
"Journal of Sex Research" under the title "Sex in
pedophiliac relationships: An empirical investigation among a
nonrepresentative group of boys."
Since the publication of Sandfort’s findings, his research has begun
to receive some reaction from American writers and researchers. Among
those who have offered criticism of his research are David Finkelhor,
author of the 1979 study Sexual Abuse of Children and other articles;
David Mrazek, co-editor of Sexually Abused Children and Their Families and
author of several articles on the topic of child sexual abuse; and the
world-famous sex researchers William Masters and Virginia Johnson, in a
textbook written with Robert Kolodny (1985). Finkelhor’s criticism
appeared in a brief review of Sandfort’s work in Forum magazine (1984);
Mrazek’s, in a review of Sandfort’s book in Contemporary Psychology
(1985). This paper will examine the types of criticism made by these
authors and their validity. In addition, it will address the question of
the ideological viewpoint from which they are made.
The criticism of Sandfort’s work can be divided into three major
categories: methodological, speculative, and moral. The moral criticisms,
which are arguably the most revealing, will be discussed last.
One of the first methodological criticisms of Sandfort’s study is
that his findings are invalid because his sample was unrepresentative.
This criticism is made by all three writers: Finkelhor, Mrazek, and
Masters, Johnson, and Kolodny. This would be a strong point against the
study if Sandfort had claimed that his sample was representative or if he
had been trying to demonstrate that most boys experience such contacts
positively. However, Sandfort readily admits in all his writings that the
sample can in no way be called representative of all boys who have sexual
contact with men. He carefully emphasizes the limitations of his research,
and states that his conclusions cannot be generalized to all man-boy
contacts, to contacts between men and girls, or to incest.
More importantly. Sandfort’s critics fail to address the question of
whether or not a representative sample was necessary for the actual
research question. Sandfort writes: "The question was whether a
sexual contact with an adult could be a positive experience for a child.
To the extent to which this research material can give a definite answer,
the question must be answered in the affirmative" (Sandfort, 1982, p.
84). Whether or not the sample was representative is beside the point.
Since the study only sought to address the question of whether such
relationships could ever be experienced positively, an unrepresentative
sample was adequate.
It is very disturbing to note the lack of any criticism of the
unrepresentative samples used by other authors who claim that such
relationships are always negative (see Adams-Tucker, 1982; Burgess, 1984;
and Peters, 1976 for examples). Where were Sandfort’s critics when
studies using rape victims, psychiatric patients, criminal cases, and
emergency room patients were used to draw conclusions about the nature of
all adult-child sexual relationships? Evidently if a study claims negative
findings then an unrepresentative sample is perfectly acceptable, but if
it claims positive findings then an unrepresentative sample renders it
invalid.
Mrazek elaborates further on the theme of biased sampling. After noting
that the boys were contacted through self-identified pedophiles who
belonged to the Netherlands Association for Sexual Reform, he argues that
"one is faced with the reality that the study sample are all men
actively involved in the pedophile movement who have strong self-interest
in the results of the research." and later complains that "the
motivation and selection of the sample" is a problem not sufficiently
addressed (Mrazek, 1985, p. 37). Mrazek seems a bit confused here - the
study sample includes the boys, not the men. The motivation of the men is
irrelevant. It should also be noted that all interviews with the boys were
conducted in private and the boys were assured that their partners would
not be allowed to see their answers to the interview questions, clearly
reducing the likelihood that the boy’s responses were influenced by the
older partner.
A second methodological criticism is that Sandfort failed to define
"negative" effects on the boys and furthermore, according to
Masters et al., that he "gave no evidence of having asked appropriate
questions to discover if these effects were present or not" (Masters,
Johnson, and Kolodny, 1985, p. 450). Unlike the complaints of
unrepresentativeness, this criticism is not irrelevant; it is simply
false. As a part of the structure of his research, Sandfort presented each
boy with a list of positive and negative emotions and behaviours. After it
was established that each boy understood these emotions and behaviours, he
was asked to indicate how often he experienced them in both his overall
relationship with the adult and in his sexual contacts with the adult. The
boy was given clear and precise means to show whether or not he had
perceived his experiences negatively. The various correlations between how
the boy felt in the relationship, how he experienced other relationships
and activities in the boy’s life and how it affected his overall sense
of well-being. In addition, Sandfort noted that when the boys were asked
to formulate negative aspects of the sexual contacts "it was often
difficult for the boys to discover any negative sides, which made it
necessary to question them rather insistently" (Sandfort, 1982, p.
64). Far from not having asked "appropriate questions," Sandfort
actually pressured the boys to come up with negative aspects.
In a similar criticism, Mrazek attacks Sandfort’s study for failing
to address the issue of whether or not these relationships might result in
the boys’ developing a sexual deviation "from more normal
heterosexuality" (Mrazek, 1985, p. 38). However, there is no evidence
that such relationships result in the younger partner becoming a
homosexual. Longitudinal studies of boys who were sexually involved with
men during adolescence (Tindall, 1978) and studies of adults who had such
experiences as boys (Bernard, 1981) have failed to find such evidence. In
addition, studies of societies where sexual relationships between men and
boys, or between boys and boys, are institutionalized have not found any
evidence of widespread adult homosexuality (Money and Ehrhardt, 1972;
Herdt, 1985). Extensive or even exclusive homosexual relations over a
period of years in childhood and adolescence do not seem to prevent boys
from becoming heterosexual adults.
Mrazek also complains that Sandfort fails to address the risk of
promiscuous early homosexual contact, including a high incidence of
genital herpes and increased risk of AIDS. It is true that Sandfort does
not address this issue, but the criticism has only limited validity. Men
who are pederasts typically do not have sexual contacts with other adult
men. The high rate of herpes and the increased risk of AIDS do not apply
to pederasts as a group as much as they apply to homosexuals (although
AIDS is now becoming more widespread in the general population). There is
a very real distinction between the two groups. In addition, there is no
indication that either the men or the boys were engaging in
"promiscuous homosexual contact; it seems to be a gratuitous
accusation thrown in by Mrazek. Finally, most of the sexual contacts
involved the partners masturbating each other; this was followed in
frequency by oral sex (most often performed on the boy by the man). Anal
contacts occurred in only a small minority of relationships. Since AIDS
seems to be transmitted mainly through exchange of body fluids in
penetrative sexual contacts, most of the man-boy sexual contacts were of
the "low-risk" type.
Master et al. offer a third methodological criticism: "Unlike
standard practice in research of this sort, no psychological tests were
administered to evaluate the boys’ emotional stability or
self-esteem..." (Masters, Johnson, and Kolodny, 1985, pp. 450-451).
In the first place, research of this sort has been very scarce; and for
that which exists, it has not been standard practice to administer
psychological tests. For example, the often-cited Finkelhor (1979) study
of college students, which is regularly invoked as "proof" that
children experience such contacts negatively, reached this conclusion by
questionaire only. The more recent study by Burgess et al. (1984) also
reached its conclusion that all such relations are harmful (actually, this
"research" simply took harm as given) without the benefit of a
single psychological test. In fact, those studies which have actually used
psychological tests, such as those of Burton (1968) in England and
Baurmann (1981) in West Germany, often come to the conclusion that such
tests show no psychological damage form the sexual relationship or
encounters. In the second place, Sandfort’s study is a psychological
evaluation. Sandfort employed an approach called the Self-Confrontation
Method based on valuation theory. This method has been used with great
success in other areas of personality research in the Netherlands and
Sandfort simply applied it to the pederastic relationships. With this
method, it was possible to determine what aspects of the boy’s
experience world were important to him; how he experienced these aspects
(positively or negatively); how frequently he experienced particular
feelings and behaviors, both positive and negative, in connection with
each area; and how each area influenced his overall well-being and
corresponded with how he would most like to feel. It is the subject (the
boy) who interprets his experience, not the researcher. As to self-esteem,
the comparison between the frequency of the feelings the boys reported in
the relationship with the man, and the frequency of the various feelings
that the boys said they would like to experience in general, showed that
the two corresponded closely. The sexual relationships with the men had a
positive overall effect on the boy’s sense of well-being and helped
bring them closer to feeling the way said they would most like to feel.
In the fourth criticism, Masters, Johnson, and Kolodny state that
"Even more unbelievably, each boy was interviewed in the home of ‘his’
pedophile with the pedophile present, without any apparent regard for the
fact that the adult’s presence would have almost assuredly prevented the
boy from voicing complaints about the way he was treated because of fear
of punishment" (Masters et al., 1985, p. 451). It is this criticism
that is unbelievable. This statement is either a knowing lie, if Masters
et al. read Sandfort’s actual study, or an irresponsible assumption, if
they did not. Sandfort stated in his book in at least three different
places that the interviews with the boys were conducted in private with
only the boy and the researcher present, in order to prevent the older
partner from influencing the boy’s answers through his presence.
Sandfort also makes this point in his article for the Journal of Sex
Research. Either Masters et al. did not bother to read Sandfort’s actual
study (in which case they have no business criticizing it) or they did,
and chose to print this claim anyway.
A check of the bibliography for Masters et al.’s textbook reveals
that the only work of Sandfort listed is his 1983 article for Alternative
Lifestyles. Since neither Sandfort’s book nor his article for the
Journal of Sex Research are listed in their bibliography, it seems that
Master’s et al. did not read either of these. As mentioned above,
Sandfort did not intend this article as a comprehensive summary of his
findings but rather as an effort to understand the meaning of the older
partner and the relationship for the younger partner. In this article,
Sandfort does not clearly state that the interviews with the boys were
conducted privately. He also does not imply that the older partner was
present during the interview. It seems that instead of referring to
Sandfort’s book or to his article in the Journal of Sex Research in
order to be sure themselves, Masters et al. chose to assume that the older
partner was present and to print this as a fact in an apparent effort to
discredit Sandfort’s research. The fact that they made such an
assumption and then failed to make any effort to check its accuracy
suggests that they are more concerned with discrediting Sandfort’s study
than with making valid and responsible criticisms of actual flaws the
study might possess.
Masters et al.’s last criticism of Sandfort’s methods was that
"no follow-up of the boys and their relationships was attempted to
discover what long-term impacts might be" (Masters et al., 1985, p.
451). Sandfort noted in his study that "considering the short period
of time elapsed during this research, it was not possible to examine the
long-range consequences of the sexual contacts" (Sandfort, 1982, p.
84). Since the original research was carried out in 1980 and all of
Sandfort’s writings appeared within the next four years, it is obvious
that not enough time had elapsed since the original study for a meaningful
long-term follow-up to take place. Significantly, Sandfort also writes
that all the boys agreed to take part in a long-term follow-up. In light
of these facts this criticism must be seen as an irrelevant accusation by
Masters, Johnson and Kolodny with the apparent intent of smearing Sandfort’s
scientific integrity and the credibility of his research. It should also
be noted that one of the major assets of Sandfort’s study is that it
deals with boys who at the time were currently involved in ongoing sexual
relationships with an adult. The sample thereby avoided the problems of
adult recall studies and of studies of boys whose relationships had been
ended by police or other social intervention.
One of Mrazek’s major criticisms is the terminology of the study. He
states that the usual labels of "perpetrator" and
"victim" are "militantly avoided" and later states
that "the reader who believes that young children should be protected
from sexual advances of adults will find the language with which the ‘researcher’
describes these relationships offensive" (Mrazek, 1985, p. 37).
Examples of the offending terminology include using the term
"partners" for the man and boy instead of "victim" and
"perpetrator," and the use of the term "making love"
for the sexual contacts.
This criticism seems to stem from a conviction that all adult-juvenile
sexual contacts are by definition abusive and exploitative. However,
avoiding the terms victim and perpetrator is not militant.
"Partners" is a relative neutral term that does not carry the
value-laden negative connotations of "victim" and
"perpetrator" nor the equally value-laden positive connotations
of the term "lover." Other writers besides Sandfort have pointed
that the offender-victim dichotomy is not always appropriate (Schultz,
1973). The fact that some boys see themselves as consenting participants
has led to the coining of the phrase "participating victim" in
an apparent effort to maintain the offender-victim distinction. As to term
"making love," Mrazek fails to point out that it is the boys
themselves who refer to the sexual contacts in this fashion - not the
researcher. One of the questions Sandfort asked of the boys was the term
they used for having sex with their adult partner, and a number of them
referred to it as "making love."
A final methodological criticism also comes from Mrazek’s review. He
argues that while Sandfort initially acknowledges his sampling bias and
warns that his findings cannot be generalized, a "logical
contortion" is necessary to reach his conclusion that "in these
cases the basis which justified the criminalizing of such contacts simply
doesn’t exist, and the result of this research suggests no other
justification for prohibiting these sexual contacts." Where is the
logical contortion? Sandfort simply points out that for consensual
contacts of the sort that he studied, the basis for criminalizing such
contacts - presumed harm to the younger partner - does not apply.
Suggesting that relationships which a boy regards as consensual and
positive should be decriminalized in no way conflicts with continued
protection of children form sexual contacts brought about by force,
coercion, deceit, or other manipulation by an adult.
Several speculative criticisms are also offered by Finkelhor and by
Masters, Johnson, and Kolodny. These criticisms do not focus on the
methodology of Sandfort’s study, but rather conjecture about possible
reasons of or rejecting his findings. Finkelhor questions whether or not
boys were honest with the researcher. He states that "children"
were selected by a group of sensitive and intellectual pedophiles and were
therefore unusual. However, he offers no explanation as to why this might
make the boys be dishonest with the researcher. What seems to be implied
here is that the boys presented a falsely positive picture of the
relationship. But why would the boys want to portray a negative
relationship as a positive one, when they presumably the ones being
"victimized?" Furthermore, how could the boys possibly lie so
consistently and thoroughly as to leave no indication of a negative
attitude towards their relationship with the older partner or of an
overall negative impact of the relationship; the behaviors they
encountered in the relationship; their relations with parents, friends,
and others; and their attitudes toward home, school, hobbies, etc.? The
suggestion that the boys were dishonest is pure speculation and seems
unlikely at best.
Finkelhor goes on to state that his own research shows that "most
kids react negatively" to sexual relations with an adult. In point of
fact, the majority of males in Finkelhor’s study rated their sexual
relation with older males as positive or neutral; less than 40% said their
encounters, as boys, had been a negative experience. Furthermore,
Finkelhor fails to mention the inherent bias in his own study, which used
a loaded questionaire seemingly designed to preclude the possibility of
reporting consensual, nonincestuous relationships with adults. Finkelhor’s
questionaire, while asking subjects about all experiences before age 12
with older partners, only asked subjects to report incestuous or
nonconsensual experiences that occurred after age 12. If any of Sandfort’s
subjects had taken this questionaire, they would not have reported their
positively experienced relationships since these did not fall into either
category.
Finkelhor’s final criticism in this category is that there is such a
fundamental asymmetry of power between adult and "child" that
kids have no real power of consent. However, many boys clearly view
themselves as willing participants and do not feel that they have been
manipulated or coerced. Even more significantly, adults reflecting back on
their relationship as children repeat this point of view (as the studies
by Bernard, 1981; Ingram, 1981; and Tindall, 1978, demonstrate). The boys
in Sandfort’s study were aware of the possible negative reactions of
parents, friends, and authorities to the relationship, yet chose to
continue it anyway. In light of these facts it seems hard to maintain the
belief that boys are somehow incapable of saying no.
Masters, Johnson and Kolodny speculate that perhaps the "boys were
so intimidated by the pedophile that they were afraid to say anything
against him" (Masters et al, 1985, p. 451). It should be noted that a
number of studies have found that there are boys who regard their
relationships as having been positive even after they became adults; and
in addition, many maintain close ties with their older partners well into
adulthood (see Bernard, 1981; Ingram, 1981; Rossman, 1976; and Tindall,
1978, for examples). This is hardly the attitude one would expect from
someone who was intimidated by, and scared of, their older partner.
Finally, the boys in Sandfort’s study did say some things against their
older partners. They felt they did encounter some negative behaviours such
as coercion, although only rarely, and also reported that sometimes they
behaved negatively towards their older partners as well. In other words,
these man-boy relationships were described by the boys just as any sort of
positive relationship between two people might be described. Although
positive behaviors predominated, petty fighting, manipulation, and
coercion on the part of both partners occurred as well. If the boys had
claimed that they always and only experienced positive behaviour, one
would have every right to suspect dishonesty, but they did not. The fact
that the boys reported both good and bad feelings and behaviours in the
relationships indicates that they were being honest when they reported
that the good far outweighed the bad in their eyes. Masters et al.’s
speculation that the boys were intimidated is not based on any interviews
with the boys in question. In light of the facts from Sandfort’s own
study and the other literature cited above, this speculation is
unwarranted.
Masters et al. conclude their criticism by explaining why adult-child
sexual encounters are always negative. They state that they agree with Dr.
Suzanne Sgroi, co-director of the St. Joseph College Institute for the
Treatment and Control of Child Sexual Abuse, who says "... The
sexually abused child may not feel abused initially, but as the child
learns what society thinks of what he has done, the child feels betrayed.
He feels he cannot trust adults or family members ... [and] has a sense of
danger, of being violated, a sense that he is not as good as he was
before" (Masters et al., 1985, p. 451). If Dr. Sgroi’s analysis is
correct, it only shows that society’s reaction makes the child feel
abused, not the sexual contact itself. In the past (and even today, to a
lesser extent), society’s attitudes toward sexual behaviors such as
masturbation and homosexuality caused untold guilt, shame and anxiety. It
is now widely recognized that the "harm" attributed to
masturbation and homosexuality came more often than not from the societal
reaction to these behaviors. In fact, it is now well understood that our
society has tended to react with horror to many types of harmless sexual
behaviors. To base a blanket condemnation of all adult-juvenile sexual
encounters on society’s reaction is erroneous.
It should also be noted that Dr. Sgroi’s analysis (at least as far as
boys are concerned) is not supported by the literature. The studies of
Bernard, Ingram, and Tindall show that many boys, far from feeling
betrayed and unable to trust people, continue close friendships with their
older partners well into adulthood and show no evidence for social or
psychological maladjustment. In addition, as stated earlier, Sandfort
found in his study that the boys were well aware of negative societal
attitudes and the possible negative reactions of parents, friends, and
authorities. However, they still viewed the relationships as positive and
felt no sense of betrayal or lowered self-esteem. All of this directly
contradicts Dr. Sgroi’s contentions.
The final category of criticism consists of moral criticisms. All three
critics of Sandfort’s study include moral criticism of one sort or
another. As stated above, it is the moral attacks that may provide some of
the best inside into the ideological viewpoints held by Sandfort’s
critics. Finkelhor claims that Sandfort’s findings do not alter the
"moral perspective" of the issue and by way of analogy says that
"there were probably slaves who loved being slaves and were not hurt
by it, but society determined that the institution was a violation of
fundamental ethical standards and abolished it" (Finkelhor, 1984, p.
8). It is hard to see how this related to the relationships studied by
Sandfort. While a slave is completely within the power of the slaveowner
and is denied basic and fundamental freedoms, there is no evidence that
the boys in question felt compelled or forced to continue the sexual
relationship. In fact, most of the boys took the initiative in sexual
contact with the adult partner at least from time to time. Some explicitly
stated that the relationship were a matter of their personal choice and
said they felt they had the right to make decisions for themselves about
whether they participated in a sexual relationship or not. Finkelhor’s
analogy seems based on the assumption that such relationships are
automatically wrong because of power differences, regardless of the boy’s
attitude toward the relationship, its impact on the boy, and whether or
not he felt coerced or forced.
A more serious moral criticism comes from Mrazek’s review. He argues
that since the sexual contacts in question were illegal in the
Netherlands, Sandfort was rationalizing criminal behaviour and was
maintaining the secrecy of a sexual offense from the boys’ families.
While this may pose an ethical dilemma, it should be pointed out that many
other studies of sexual behavior are open to the same criticism. For
example, Kinsey interviewed many adolescents and prepubescent boys about
their sexual behavior for his work Sexual Behavior in the Human Male
(1948), recognized as a landmark in sex research. However, sexual
behavior, while legally acceptable in adults, is ground for legally
judging an adolescent or child a delinquent. Kinsey himself pointed out
that from a specific calculation from his data, some 86% of adolescent
boys in America were sexual delinquents. Should Kinsey have reported all
these boys to the authorities as sexual delinquents? Later researchers
into adolescent sexual behavior, such as Sorensen (1973), can be attacked
on the same grounds. Researchers investigating homosexuality in the 60s
and 70s are also open to the same criticism - nearly all states in the
U.S. had laws against homosexual behaviour in the 1960s and many still
have today. Should these researchers have turned their subjects in to the
police for prosecution because they were violating the law by
participating in homosexual encounters with other men? What about
psychiatrists and psychologists with homosexual patients? Should they have
reported their patients to the police for prosecution?
This ethical dilemma is by no means an easy one to resolve. However,
Sandfort was faced with a group of boys who stated that they enjoyed their
relationship with the older partner and the sexual contacts involved. In
addition, in 7 of the 25 cases the boy’s parents were aware of the
relationship and accepted it. Would it have helped these boys in the short
or the long term if their relationships had been reported to the police
and broken up? Mrazek also fails to note that a very different climate
exists in the Netherlands than in the U.S. regarding adult-child sex.
Police are reluctant to prosecute when there is no evidence of coercion
and when the youth involved is at least 12 years old. In addition, an
ongoing public debate on the age of consent has even included appearances
by children and their adult partners on national radio to discuss their
relationships. In such a climate, the issue of reporting does not carry
the same weight as it does in the U.S., where professionals are required
by law in many states to report such relationships. It should also be
noted that in the Netherlands, clinicians are not under any legal
obligation to report crimes committed by their clients (with the exception
of assaults on someone’s life), unlike the situation in many states in
the U.S.
The inclusion of a moral perspective to condemn man-boy sex is perhaps
most surprising in the case of the textbook by Masters, Johnson and
Kolodny. Earlier in their text, Masters et al. pointed to the
misconceptions engendered and maintained by moral arguments. For instance,
we are informed that the Catholic Church continues to insist that
"masturbation is an intrinsically and seriously disordered act"
and that this "unnatural act" came to be described as
"self-abuse", "defilement of the flesh," and
"self-pollution." In the case of homosexuality, Masters at al.
tell us that this form of sexual activity has been described in judicial
decisions from the 1970s as "loathsome and disgusting," "
grossly repugnant," "degenerate," "foul," and
"immoral, lewd, and obscene." They quote one psychiatrist who,
in 1970, described homosexuality as "a dread dysfunction, malignant
in character, which has risen to epidemic proportions" (Masters et
al., 1985, p. 407).
Thus, it is surprising that Masters et al. ask in reference to man-boy
relationships, "is an inherently abusive exploitive relationship ‘positive’
under any circumstances?" (Masters et al., 1985, p. 451). They are
aware of how such loaded terminology and sweeping condemnations of whole
categories of sexual behaviour prevented an objective understanding of
masturbation and homosexuality, and they are aware that a clearer
understanding was attained only through a deliberate disregard for such
moral condemnation in favour of concentration on empirical research. But
ignoring this lesson in the case of their own evaluation of man-boy sex,
Masters et al. have strengthened the impression that science is only
welcome so long as it fits in with the prevailing ideologies. They are
evidently perfectly comfortable in condemning all man-boy sexual
relationships as "inherently abusive and exploitive," even
though they reject this sort of claim as it was employed in the past
against other types of sexual behavior.
The moral criticism of Sandfort’s work thus appear to all be guided
by a particular set of beliefs about adult-juvenile sex. These beliefs
center primarily on the idea that such a relationship are by their nature
abusive and exploitive. They include a belief that the younger is
automatically incapable of consent, because of the adult’s position of
greater power; a belief that the adult inevitably exploits the younger
partner for his own needs, without concern for the youngster’s own
feelings and desires; and a belief that such relationship always or nearly
always have negative consequences for the younger partner. More subtly,
but just as significantly, they also seem to be based at least in part on
a belief that deviations from the adult heterosexual norm are
automatically suspect (witness Mrazek’s comment on "more normal
heterosexuality").
The methodological criticisms of Sandfort’s work are variously
distorted, irrelevant, or just plain false; the speculative criticisms are
either irrelevant or else biased by their complete failure to consider
contrary findings in the literature; and the moral criticism show a
dogmatic adherence to the belief that all adult-juvenile sexual relations
are by definition abusive, exploitive, and harmful. It seems that the
taboo against juvenile sexuality and particularly against adult-juvenile
sex is still so strong that research which fails to support the prevailing
ideology must be attacked and discredited, regardless of its actual
validity. It remains to be seen whether scientific objectivity can prevail
against the need to defend the current dogma on man-boy sexual contacts in
particular and adult-juvenile sex in general.